News

Autism 'Epidemic' Denied, Ascribed to Diagnosis Shift


 

Claims of an autism “epidemic” are not backed by the data, which show evidence that diagnostic substitution accounts for reported increases in the disorder and that those increases are still short of epidemiologic estimates, according to one researcher.

Paul T. Shattuck, Ph.D., a research associate at the University of Wisconsin, Madison, conducted an analysis of disability data from the Department of Education for U.S. children aged 6–11 years from all 50 states and the District of Columbia from 1984 to 2003.

The data included the numbers of children categorized with any of 13 disabilities. He then used data from the U.S. Census to calculate incidence rates (Pediatrics 2006;117:1028–37).

He found that, according to logistic regression models, reported autism incidence met three criteria he established to determine that diagnostic substitution does, indeed, play a role: First, the nationwide incidence of reported autism has gone up, while the reported national incidence of other special-education categories (including mental retardation [MR] and learning disabilities [LD]) has gone down.

Second, within most states, increases in autism diagnoses are correlated with decreases in the number of children diagnosed with MR or LD. And third, the trajectories for the prevalence of various learning disorders have gone down at the same time that the administrative incidence of autism has gone up.

For the entire United States, the mean reported incidence of autism in children aged 3–10 years had gone from less than 1 case per 1,000 in 1994 to approximately 3 per 1,000 in 2003. However, this incidence rate was still short of the lower threshold of a reference range of expected incidence calculated by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Thus, despite the marked increase in reported national incidence of autism over that time period, the incidence rate still did not meet the criteria required to be an epidemic, Dr. Shattuck wrote.

Moreover, the reported rates of learning disability and mental retardation decreased during the same period (odds ratio 0.98 and 0.97, respectively), while the overall incidence of all disabilities declined (by a non-statistically significant measure), discounting any claim that increased autism rates are a function of increased special education enrollment overall.

Dr. Shattuck also reported that LD and MR rates had declined in most states. The only states with increases in the learning disabilities category as well as autism were Oklahoma and Pennsylvania, and the only states with increases in mental retardation and autism were California, Michigan, New Jersey, and West Virginia. Increases in both disabilities were also found in the District of Columbia. He also observed that the historical trajectories for MR and LD prevalence were lower between 1984 and 1993 than between 1994 and 2003, with the advent of the autism diagnosis.

Dr. Shattuck noted that diagnostic substitution is suspected by many clinicians because the diagnostic criteria for autism have been expanded over the last few decades and that this phenomenon has been documented previously in special-education enrollment. He pointed out that the category of LD grew by 198% between 1976 and 1992, and the MR category shrank by 41%.

In an editorial accompanying the report, Craig J. Newschaffer, Ph.D., of Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, conceded that diagnostic substitution is one factor that might contribute to increased autism prevalence. But Dr. Newschaffer added that not only the directions of classification trends but also their magnitudes should be incorporated into any statistical analysis thereof (Pediatrics 2006;117:1436–7).

Dr. Chris P. Johnson, medical director of the Village of Hope Center for Children With Disabilities at the University of Texas, San Antonio, and cochair of the American Academy of Pediatrics Autism Panel, offered qualified agreement with the theory of a diagnostic shift. She noted that before 1991, there was no label schools could use to give special education services to autistic children, other than MR, speech delayed, and LD. The “autism” label became available in 2003 with passage of the Improving Education Results for Children With Disabilities Act.

Moreover, because more services are often available to children with autism, including year-round schooling, behavioral management training, a home trainer, and funding for parental respite and/or recreation, parents and professionals may view the autism label as preferable to MR or LD. But despite this, “I certainly do not think [diagnostic substitution] is the only reason, and it may not even be the main reason, for the apparent rise in prevalence,” she added.

Good intentions on the part of administrators may also play a role, she explained. “I think when there's truly uncertainty as to whether the diagnosis in a particular child is autism or something close to it, a label of autism is sometimes given in a compassionate effort to entitle the child to additional services, which will also, in turn, benefit the family.”

Pages

Recommended Reading

School-Based Project Improves Girls' Coping Skills
MDedge Psychiatry
Antidepressants Raise Suicide Risk, Data Show
MDedge Psychiatry
Depression Contagion: Parents Can Affect Children
MDedge Psychiatry
Children With Anxiety, Depression More Likely to Use Ecstasy
MDedge Psychiatry
Tool Helps Spot Bipolar Prodrome in Children : Questionnaire asks patients to rate 39 symptoms that can emerge before the first manic episode.
MDedge Psychiatry
Dearth of Evidence in Guiding Tx Of Bipolar Depression in Teens
MDedge Psychiatry
Quetiapine May Help Manage Depression in Bipolar Adolescents
MDedge Psychiatry
Children Suffer Long Term When Parent Has a Stroke
MDedge Psychiatry
Olfactory Deficits May Be a Signal for Early Psychosis
MDedge Psychiatry
Lamotrigine Effective Add-On for Seizures : Adjunctive treatment reduces frequency of primary generalized tonic-clonic seizures.
MDedge Psychiatry